PDF Raising a Modern Frontier Boy: Directing a Film and a Life with My Son

Free download. Book file PDF easily for everyone and every device. You can download and read online Raising a Modern Frontier Boy: Directing a Film and a Life with My Son file PDF Book only if you are registered here. And also you can download or read online all Book PDF file that related with Raising a Modern Frontier Boy: Directing a Film and a Life with My Son book. Happy reading Raising a Modern Frontier Boy: Directing a Film and a Life with My Son Bookeveryone. Download file Free Book PDF Raising a Modern Frontier Boy: Directing a Film and a Life with My Son at Complete PDF Library. This Book have some digital formats such us :paperbook, ebook, kindle, epub, fb2 and another formats. Here is The CompletePDF Book Library. It's free to register here to get Book file PDF Raising a Modern Frontier Boy: Directing a Film and a Life with My Son Pocket Guide.

Bierman et al. While promising, it will be important for the present findings to be replicated and extended to address a number of limitations. For example, our sample came primarily from an affluent, suburban sample. This sample nonetheless included a broad enough range of incomes that income was predictive of self-directed EF, and the relationship between less-structured time and self-directed EF held even when controlling for income. However, less-structured time may be especially beneficial to children in safe, quiet, resource-rich environments, so it will be important to test whether it differentially relates to self-direction in more impoverished environments.

In addition, although the current test of the relationship between less-structured time and self-directed EFs emerged from a targeted hypothesis, we conducted multiple post-hoc exploratory analyses to explore the relationship between specific activities and self-directed control, which are not ideal conditions for statistical inference.

Another limitation of the present study relates to our constructions of less-structured and structured time, which are imprecise, and most likely fail to capture important differences across activities. The broad, standardized definitions of structured and less-structured time adopted in this study e. In the present study, trips to museums, libraries, and sporting events are each classified as less-structured, but may vary in relative structure.

That is, a typical library visit, where children may select their own sections to browse and books to check out, may involve much less structure and more self-directed time than a typical sporting event, where attention is largely directed toward the action on the field or court. Even those activities that seem less-structured by definition, such as free play, can quickly become more structured when adults, older siblings, or peers impose additional rules or criteria. Indeed, many programmatic interventions have highlighted the importance of some structure to improve the quality of children's play and other learning experiences, and produce benefits Schweinhart et al.

We note however, that even though our classification system based on the existing literature does not capture these variations in exactly how structured various activities are, our primary finding of the relationship between less-structured time and self-directed EF holds across analyses dropping potentially more difficult-to-interpret classifications e. To generate a more precise estimate of the amount of time children spend pursuing activities in a self-directed way, one would ideally assess child time directly, possibly by supplementing parent-reported child time use data with direct observation.

One possibility along these lines could be to employ experience sampling techniques Miller, , where parents are frequently queried via cell phone or another mobile device throughout the day and asked to provide specific detail about their child's activities in the moment. Such methods would also minimize the need to rely on a parent's memory for their child's daily activities and experiences. We view our work as providing an important starting point for this kind of more time-intensive study of children's time outside of formal schooling and its relationship to their self-directed EF.

In addition, although we have identified links between child time use and self-directed EF, we are unable to draw firm conclusions about whether the observed relationships were driven by activities occurring in the week preceding the test session as has been observed in other domains, e. This finding could reflect the importance of the combination of recent and distal experiences, or simply the greater robustness of using a composite measure.

Therefore, while we have posited that less-structured experiences allow children to practice self-directed, goal-oriented behavior, producing benefits over time, we cannot discount the possibility that observed linkages may have been driven by recent experiences which increased self-directed behavior. In either scenario, regular participation in less-structured activities would yield benefits.

Future investigations of the relationship between self-directed control and less-structured time would also benefit from the inclusion of additional measures of self-directed control, which more closely approximate real-world child behaviors. This process may benefit from the development and validation of new measures of self-directed control in children. Establishing effects using tasks tapping other forms of self-direction would also ensure generalizability. For instance, in the present study, time in less-structured activities such as family outings may have benefitted verbal fluency performance in a specific way, by fostering the development of more well-organized semantic networks, rather than by more generally improving children's abilities to generate their own rules for how and when to employ EFs to achieve their goals.

This alternative account cannot explain the full pattern of results in the link between less-structured time and self-directed EF e. The findings of the current study are consistent with previous research in showing a link between children's experiences and EF Lillard and Else-quest, ; Diamond et al.

However, while the current study found specific effects of time use on self-directed but not externally-driven EF, previous research found effects of training and preschool interventions on externally-driven EF e. There are several possible reasons for this discrepancy. First, previous training studies that have shown benefits for externally-driven EF have specifically trained children on aspects of externally-driven EF e. Likewise, while preschool and other interventions include a wide variety of experiences, they likely include considerable practice with externally-driven EF.

In contrast, we hypothesize that less-structured time primarily affords children practice with self-directed EF, and thus may not transfer to improving externally-driven EF. Second, it is possible that differences between the current versus previous studies could be accounted for by differences between the externally-driven EF tasks they employed. Many previous studies that have found effects of interventions on externally-driven EF used task-switching or working memory span tasks e.

It may be that specific aspects of externally-driven EF are more sensitive to children's experiences, or that specific tasks are more sensitive to individual differences in general due to better psychometric properties Future research using a more comprehensive battery of EF tasks could address these possibilities. Another key difference between our study and such prior research is the correlational nature of our study, which supports at least two alternatives to the interpretation that how children spend their leisure time shapes their EF. First, children with better self-directed EFs may engage in or be encouraged to engage in less-structured activities more often.

Likewise, children with poorer self-directed control may be more likely to engage in structured activities. Alternatively, the observed relationship between less-structured time and self-directed control may be driven by a third, unmeasured variable. Although we have attempted to control for some characteristics that might influence both time spent in less-structured activities and verbal fluency, such as household income, we have not controlled for other possibilities, such as parent EF and child's fluid intelligence which we did not assess. However, we did control for child vocabulary an index of crystallized intelligence , which may serve as a proxy for fluid intelligence in testing relationships with EF, given that EF fully mediates the correlation between crystallized and fluid intelligence in 7-years-old Brydges et al.

Moreover, such factors might be expected to predict both children's self-directed EF and their externally-driven EF Ardila et al. Similar issues have been raised in interpreting links observed between children's EF and pretend play: rather than reflecting a uniquely causal role for pretend play in EF, EF may instead play a causal role in supporting pretend play, or pretend play may be one of many activities promoting EF development in young children Lillard et al. An important direction for future work lies in establishing the directionality of relationships between child time use and self-directed EF, through experimental manipulation.

Longitudinal studies could provide the first step toward establishing directionality. Specifically, if time spent in less-structured activities prospectively predicts change in self-directed EF, this would suggest that less-structured time may play a causal role in the development of self-directed EF. If, on the other hand, self-directed EF prospectively predicts changes in the amount of time children spend in less-structured activities, this would suggest that self-directed EF may play a causal role in children's time use e. While such longitudinal studies could thus provide important information about temporal precedence, this information is not sufficient evidence of causality e.

Thus, future research using experimental manipulations of time spent in less-structured activities is necessary to definitively test causality.

Contribute to This Page

Although this kind of work is ambitious, and poses challenges, it could be used to inform more targeted laboratory-based training studies. Finally, we hope that future explorations of the relationship between child time use and developing self-directed EFs will inform a wider question: specifically, whether societal shifts in child time use over the past 50 years have influenced development.

Hours formerly devoted to less-structured, social play have been replaced by media time Vandewater et al. Some commentators have warned that these changes have been to the detriment of children e. Others have argued that children benefit more from regular skill practice in structured settings e. Our findings indicate that during children's time outside of formal schooling, participation in less structured activities may benefit the development of self-directed EFs, while participation in structured activities may hinder the development of self-directed EFs.

Thorough testing of this hypothesis remains an important direction for future work. Jane E. Barker, Andrei D. Semenov, and Yuko Munakata contributed to the development of the study hypothesis. All authors contributed to study design. Barker performed the data analysis and drafted the manuscript with input from Yuko Munakata. Critical revisions were contributed by Hannah R.

Snyder, Laura Michaelson, and Yuko Munakata. All authors discussed the results, implications, and literature, and approved the final version of the manuscript for submission. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The authors wish to thank Julia Stadele for her assistance in coding verbal fluency data and coordinating research subjects, Joe Brill for his assistance in coding time diary data, and Ryan Guild for his helpful comments on manuscript revisions.

Our definition of structured activities is also consistent with past studies of structured leisure time, which have excluded time spent in school e. However, we have classified reading and television as less-structured time, and studying as structured time, in keeping with other studies e.

Original Research ARTICLE

For example, if parents who left fewer cells blank in the time use survey had children with higher self-directed EF, this could have contributed to the observed correlation between less-structured time and self-directed EF, since parents who left fewer cells blank might report more time in less-structured activities. However, this interpretation rests on the validity and sensitivity of the typicality measure, which is unknown.

Parent-reported typicality is at least internally consistent with parent-reported time use. The Flanker task can be sensitive to minor variations in stimulus parameters Paquet, and intervention dosage in adults Liu-Ambrose et al. Failures to find effects of interventions have also been attributed in part to the task's sensitivity to practice effects in pre-post measure designs as discussed in Rueda et al.

Abwender, D.

Qualitative analysis of verbal fluency output: review and comparison of several scoring methods. Assessment 8, — Agnew, R. Leisure and delinquency. CrossRef Full Text. Albertson, K. Holding in mind conflicting information: pretending, working memory, and executive control. Ardila, A.

Correlation between intelligence test scores and executive function measures. Bavelier, D. Children, wired: for better and worse. Neuron 67, — Bergman Nutley, S. Gains in fluid intelligence after training non-verbal reasoning in 4-year-old children: a controlled, randomized study.


  1. Top 50 modern movie documentaries?
  2. Marketing Concept & Intercultural Communication: Development of a marketing strategy primarily based on intercultural communication aspects (applying Hofstede’s, Trompenaars’ and Hall’s studies).
  3. Abbas Kiarostami, Iranian director whose films knew no borders?
  4. The Adventure Train Travels Through Africa;
  5. 12222 SUNDANCE FILM FESTIVAL: 112 FEATURES ANNOUNCED.
  6. How To Deal With Long Distance Relationships?
  7. Login using.

Berns, G. Short- and long-term effects of a novel on connectivity in the brain. Brain Connect. Best, J. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Bianchi, S. Changing Rhythms of American Family Life.

ASA Rose Series. Bierman, K. Executive functions and school readiness intervention: impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program. Birn, R.

Raising a Modern Frontier Boy: Directing a Film and a Life with My Son by John Grooters

Neural systems supporting lexical search guided by letter and semantic category cues: a self-paced overt response fMRI study of verbal fluency. Neuroimage 49, — Blair, C. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending Head Start. Bodrova, E. Vygotsky and Montessori: one dream, two visions.

Montessori Life 15, 30— Tools of the Mind. Columbus, OH: Pearson.

This site is not available in your region

Braver, T. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, and J. Brocki, K. Executive functions in children aged 6 to a dimensional and developmental study. Bryck, R. On the role of verbalization during task set selection: switching or serial order control? Brydges, C. A unitary executive function predicts intelligence in children.

Intelligence 40, — Cameron, C. Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to kindergarten achievement. Carlson, S.

Join Kobo & start eReading today

Evidence for a relation between executive function and pretense representation in preschool children. Chatham, C. Pupillometric and behavioral markers of a developmental shift in the temporal dynamics of cognitive control. Chevalier, N. Switch detection in preschoolers' cognitive flexibility. Child Psychol. Chrysikou, E. Dissociable brain states linked to common and creative object use.

Brain Mapp. Chua, A. Battle Hymn of the Tiger Mother. New York, NY: Penguin. Connor, C. First graders' literacy and self-regulation gains: the effect of individualizing student instruction. Costafreda, S. A systematic review and quantitative appraisal of fMRI studies of verbal fluency: role of the left inferior frontal gyrus. Daneman, M. Working memory and language comprehension: a meta-analysis. Dennis, M. Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders.

Diamond, A. Activities and programs that improve children's executive functions. Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science , — Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children years old. D'Ostilio, K. Dissociation between unconscious motor response facilitation and conflict in medial frontal areas. Drane, D. Structured cueing on a semantic fluency task differentiates patients with temporal versus frontal lobe seizure onset.

Epilepsy Behav. Eccles, J. Student council, volunteering, basketball, or marching band: what kind of extracurricular involvement matters? Elias, C. Self-regulation in young children: is there a role for sociodramatic play? Early Child. Ellamil, M. Evaluative and generative modes of thought during the creative process. Neuroimage 59, — Engle, R.


  • Bareback Gay Sex Orgy With The 18-year-old Russian Lads?
  • Family Forever;
  • Like A Child.
  • ‘Triple Frontier’ Review: A Thriller that Becomes a Plodding Parable – Variety;
  • SET-UP.
  • Raising a Modern Frontier Boy: Directing a Film and a Life with My Son.
  • The British Imperial Century, 1815–1914: A World History Perspective (Critical Issues in World and International History).
  • Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. Eriksen, C. Information processing in visual search: a continuous flow conception and experimental results. Farah, M. Childhood poverty: specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Res. Fisher, A. Effects of a physical education intervention on cognitive function in young children: randomized controlled pilot study. BMC Pediatr. Fletcher, A.

    Structured leisure activities in middle childhood: links to well-being. Community Psychol. Forstmann, B. Internally generated and directly cued task sets: an investigation with fMRI. Overview Readers reviews 0 Product Details. Your rating Click on the stars for rating. Your review Headline characters remaining Review characters remaining.

    Add to shopping bag. Tintin et la Lune - Double album Copyright - Antoine Online - All rights reserved. Login Register. Screenplay: Mark Boal, J. Camera color, widescreen : Roman Vasyanov. Editor: Ron Patane. Music: Disasterpeace. Amy Stoch and Hal Landon Jr.

    My Shopping Bag

    Stoch and Landon Jr. Bennett will round out the [ Watch Kristen Bell reprise her role as the titular character in Season 4 [ Chandor's drama about five ex-military pals who reunite to rip off a drug lord is an action thriller that turns into a plodding parable of greed. By Owen Gleiberman. Director: J. Previous video Next video. Close Menu. Variety Intelligence Platform.